Having more ‘sweet dreams’ may help with your ‘sweet tooth’

Sweet tooth

Sweet tooth

Having more ‘sweet dreams’ may help with your ‘sweet tooth’10 January 2018

“The key to stopping yourself snacking could be as simple as getting into bed,” reports the Mail Online. Researchers found that people who usually sleep less than 7 hours a night can increase their sleep time, and this may be linked to them eating less sugary food.

Previous studies linked insufficient sleep to an increased chance of obesity and poor diet. However, this small study, designed to explore whether education about sleep could help people sleep longer, was not set up to show whether improved sleep could change diet or aid weight loss.

The study of 42 volunteers, all a healthy weight and mostly young, found that education about sleep could increase participants’ time in bed by almost an hour, and sleep duration by 21 minutes, compared to the start of the study. People who took part in sleep education also reported reducing their sugar consumption more than those who had no sleep education. However, their overall calorie intake, energy expenditure and body mass index did not change during the 4-week study.

We don’t know whether better sleep will help you to lose weight, if that’s what you want to do. But good sleep is great for overall mental and physical health. And as a 2016 survey reported, a third of British people surveyed reported not getting enough sleep on a regular basis.

Adults should sleep around 7 to 9 hours a night. Find out more about ways to get a good night’s sleep.

Where did the story come from?

The researchers who carried out the study were from King’s College London and Queen’s University in Belfast. The study was published in the peer-reviewed American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, and was funded by the British Nutrition Foundation. The study was published on an open-access basis so it is free to read online.

Most of the UK media coverage suggested the study had shown longer sleep helps people to eat less and lose weight, which was not the case. For example, the Mail Online carried a fairly accurate report of the study, but without exploring whether people actually ate less sugar – or whether they just said they did. And the Daily Mirror claimed: “One extra hour in bed can make you healthier and drop your weight,” although the study did not find weight loss among the people taking part. All of the participants were a healthy weight at the start of the study in any case.

What kind of research was this?

This was a randomised controlled pilot study. Randomised controlled trials are the best type of study to see whether a treatment works. However, because this was a pilot study, it was set up mainly to see if the treatment (sleep education) was feasible, rather than whether it could affect people’s diet. That is, is it possible through education and training, to encourage people in the intervention group to sleep longer than they used to? (The answer would appear to be only for a small minority).

We would need to see bigger, longer-term studies with more reliable outcomes to know whether sleep education can help with weight loss.

What did the research involve?

Researchers recruited 42 volunteers via King’s College London, many of whom were students. They all had a healthy body mass index, no serious illnesses and self-reported that they regularly slept less than 7 hours during the working week.

Everyone went through a battery of biomedical tests and wore an actigraph (a device that tracks physical movement) on their wrist for a week to measure sleep. They also recorded their diet in a food diary for 7 days. After that, they were randomly assigned to either the control group (which received no sleep education) or the sleep education group. Those in the sleep education group had personalised education about their sleep routine and agreed to change certain behaviours, including an agreed bedtime.

After 3 weeks, everyone had their sleep monitored again for a week by wrist actigraph and repeated the food diary. They also repeated other biomedical tests, which included:

resting metabolic rate assessment

measurement of waist, height and weight

total energy expenditure

blood tests to look at liver function, glucose level and cholesterol level

blood pressure

The sleep measurements, which were backed up by a sleep diary, looked at total time in bed, sleep period, sleep duration (the time spent sleeping during the sleep period) and quality of sleep (such as the proportion of time in bed spent asleep, time taken to get to sleep and waking during the night).

What were the basic results?

There were 4 drop outs from the study (3 due to time constraints and 1 because of technical problems with the sleep monitor). Of those who had sleep education, 86% increased their average time in bed and 50% increased their sleep duration.

However, only 3 of the 21 people who had sleep education actually increased their sleep to the recommended 7 to 9 hours a night weekly average.

The average difference from baseline, compared with the control group, was:

56 minutes longer time in bed (range 31 minutes to 1 hour 21 minutes)

52 minutes longer sleep period (range 27 minutes to 1 hour 17 minutes) – the total time spent asleep over the course of a night

32 minutes longer sleep duration (range 11 minutes to 54 minutes) – this measured the length of individual episodes of sleep over the course of a night

Subjective reporting of sleep quality, however, including the percentage of time in bed spent asleep, went down for those who had sleep education.

The dietary diary resulted in 9 measures of type of food intake (such as protein, carbohydrates, sugars and fibre) and 2 measures of diet quality. Of these, the only ones that showed a difference between the groups compared to baseline were:

free sugars (sugars added to food or present in sugary food such as juice or honey), 10.3g lower in the sleep extension group than the control group per day

protein consumption remained 4.5g higher per day in the sleep extension group

the sleep extension group eating more in line with UK dietary guidelines, largely because they ate less sugar

Although there was a slight drop in total energy intake (reported in some of the UK media), the study was not big enough for us to be sure it was not a chance finding.

How did the researchers interpret the results?

The researchers said their study showed “a personalised behavioural consultation targeting sleep hygiene is a feasible lifestyle intervention” that can be used to test the effects of longer sleep.

They said their “pilot investigation” of diet “indicates that adherence to advice to extend sleep may reduce free sugars intake”. They say it is possible that “participating in the sleep intervention had driven changes in the [sleep education] group’s dietary reporting” – meaning that the group attending for education on sleep may have consciously or unconsciously under-reported the amount of sugar they ate, because they wanted to seem healthy.

An alternate explanation, they say, is that sugar cravings may be increased by lack of sleep, or that spending more time asleep limits the time people are able to eat snacks.

Conclusion

A decent night’s sleep makes a big difference to how you feel and how much energy you have. Lack of sleep and poor sleep habits have been linked to health problems in the past. This study suggests that it may be possible to improve sleep by education in “sleep hygiene” – the things you can do to improve your chances of sleeping well.

The study provides some evidence that this might affect what you eat. But the evidence is not particularly strong, and could be a result of people inaccurately reporting what they ate. The study was not designed to be big enough to capture changes in people’s diet, and only found a major change in “free sugars” – sugar added to foods such as biscuits and sweets, or present in large quantities in fruit juice or honey.

It’s encouraging that the study found a positive effect from education to help people sleep better. However, if you want to lose weight, it’s likely you will need to do more about it than “sleep yourself slim”. A properly designed weight loss diet can help – see your GP for more advice.

Sleep hygiene advice for a good night’s sleep includes:

keep regular sleep hours

create a restful bedtime environment

exercise regularly – but not straight before bed

cut down on caffeine

don’t eat just before going to bed

10 Tips for Better Sleep

A good night’s sleep is critical to anyone’s health. As the seasons transition, so do sleep patterns. Summer means kids are out of school, adults are taking days off from work to enjoy much needed vacations, the days are longer and everyone’s traveling, so it’s easy to miss out on sleep. Here are 10 easy tips that you can easily implement to ensure you’re well rested, healthy, and ready to take on the world!

10 Tips for Better Sleep

From Visually.

Does having a ‘sense of purpose’ in life help you sleep better?

Sleep

Sleep

“Sense of purpose aids sleep, US scientists find,” The Guardian reports on a new study that explored the relationship between having a sense of purpose in life and quality of sleep in older adults.

The study analysed data from 800 older adults with an average age of 80 in the US.

Researchers found that generally, having a greater sense of purpose in life was associated with better quality of sleep, as well as a decreased likelihood of sleep disorders such as sleep apnoea and restless leg syndrome.

Although these are interesting findings, it’s not possible to rule out the influence of other factors.

The fairly abstract concept of “sense of purpose” may be influenced by various health and lifestyle factors, such as levels of physical activity and mental health problems, and these may all in turn affect quality of sleep.

But this study wasn’t able to pull out all of the intricacies of this complex relationship.

Problems with sleep are more common in the UK than most people realise, but there are proven ways to help combat insomnia.

As for having a “sense of purpose”, research has shown that volunteering your time for a cause or charity you believe in can help improve your mental wellbeing.

Read more about how giving can improve your wellbeing.

Where did the story come from?

The study was carried out by researchers from Northwestern University in the US, and was funded by the National Institute on Aging Grant Numbers and the Illinois Department of Health.

It was published in the peer-reviewed journal Sleep Science and Practice. It’s available on an open access basis and is free to read online.

The UK media coverage around this research was generally balanced and well reported.

What kind of research was this?

This analysis of data from two cohort studies set out to explore the relationship between having a sense of purpose in life and quality of sleep.

Previous research has suggested that having a sense of purpose in life could protect against several negative health outcomes, one being sleep disturbances. Sleep disturbance is known to be more common among older adults.

Studies have also observed the prevalence of sleep disturbance to be higher among African Americans than white people. The researchers wanted to investigate this further.

Cohort studies are useful for looking at an association between an exposure and an outcome. But the study design means it isn’t possible to fully rule out the influence of other confounding factors and prove that a purpose in life directly leads to better sleep.

What did the research involve?

The data sample for this analysis was taken from two ongoing Chicago-based cohort studies: the Minority Aging Research Study (MARS) and the Rush Memory and Aging Project (MAP).

MARS is a study of risk factors for cognitive decline that recruits older African Americans who haven’t had a diagnosis of dementia.

MAP aims to look at the brain changes associated with ageing and cognitive decline. It recruited older adults of mostly white ethnicity (88%) without a diagnosis of dementia who agreed to annual clinical assessments, as well as brain autopsy after they died.

The analysis included 825 older adults with an average age of 79.

Purpose in life was measured at the start of the studies using a modified 10-item assessment derived from the Ryff and Keyes’ Scales of Psychological Well-Being, a tool used to assess sense of purpose.

As part of the assessment, individuals were asked to respond to statements like “I feel good when I think of what I’ve done in the past and what I hope to do in the future”, and “Some people wander aimlessly through life, but I am not one of them”.

Participants used a five-point scale for their responses, ranging from 1 strongly disagree to 5 strongly agree. Higher scores were used to indicate higher levels of purpose in life.

Sleep quality and symptoms of potential sleep disorders were assessed using a 32-step questionnaire derived from the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI), the Berlin Questionnaire, and the Mayo Sleep Questionnaire (MSQ). The questionnaire was given to participants at the end of each annual visit.

The PSQI assessed sleep quality, specifically looking at how long it takes to fall asleep, sleep duration, and how much you actually sleep during the night.

The Berlin questionnaire assessed risk of sleep apnoea, and the MSQ assessed the presence of restless leg syndrome and REM behaviour disorder, where dreams are acted out (for example, through sleepwalking or shouting out).

Sleep data was collected at baseline and follow-up points at the end of the first, second and third year.

The researchers analysed any links with purpose in life, adjusting for potential confounders like age, sex, race and years of education.

Changes in quality of sleep over the course of the two-year study were also taken into account.

What were the basic results?

Out of all the 825 respondents, at the beginning of the study 42% were at high risk of sleep apnoea, 23.6% were exhibiting symptoms of restless leg syndrome, and 7% had symptoms of REM behaviour disorder.

Higher levels of purpose in life were associated with better sleep quality. Over a one-year period, improved sleep quality was reported in people with a higher “purpose of life”.

Increased levels of purpose in life were associated with a decreased risk of sleep apnoea (odds ratio [OR] 0.630, 95% confidence interval [CI] 0.454 to 0.875). This association continued during the first and second follow-up assessment.

Purpose in life wasn’t significantly associated with symptoms of restless leg syndrome. But at year one of follow-up, it was associated with a decreased likelihood of having possible restless leg syndrome (OR 0.524, 95% CI 0.361 to 0.762).

Purpose in life wasn’t significantly associated with REM behaviour disorder at baseline or years one, two and three of follow-up.

How did the researchers interpret the results?

The researchers concluded that, “In a biracial sample of over 800 older adults, the present findings provide support for the hypothesis that purpose in life is related to sleep quality, with indications that it could be a potentially useful clinical tool for assessing older adults.”

They added: “We found that higher levels of purpose in life at baseline predicted better sleep quality at baseline, as well as increased change in sleep quality over a one-year period, a finding that is consistent with previous studies.”

Conclusion

This study explored the relationship between having a sense of purpose in life and sleep quality and sleep disorders.

Researchers found generally, having a greater sense of purpose in life was associated with better quality of sleep and a decreased likelihood of sleep disorders like sleep apnoea and restless leg syndrome.

The researchers suggest this may be down to people having better overall physical and mental health.

Although these are plausible hypotheses, there are a few points to note. As with the majority of cohort studies, it isn’t possible to prove cause and effect and fully rule out the influence of other health, lifestyle and personal factors in the associations.

For example, having a healthy lifestyle can have an impact on quality of sleep. Drinking too much alcohol, smoking, not getting enough exercise, and mental health problems may reduce the chances of having a good night’s sleep.

And it’s difficult to know the exact impact of having less of a sense of purpose in life on sleep quality. This is a fairly abstract concept that may have various external influences this study wasn’t able to fully explore.

The length of time a person has felt a particular way may also have an effect. For example, the effect on sleep may not be the same in someone who’s felt they have no purpose in life for a long time compared with someone who’s recently been under acute stress.

It would be interesting to conduct this study in young adults to see if the findings are similar. There may also be different possible influences on sleep, such as different dietary factors (like sugary drink consumption) or increased screen use, in other populations.

Lack of sleep linked to negative thinking

Sleep and negative feelings

Sleep and negative feelings

“Feeling anxious? Go to bed earlier: Getting more sleep really can calm the mind,” reports the Mail Online.

However, if you’re more of a “glass half-empty” sort of person, the headline could have read “feeling anxious affects your sleep” – which is an equally valid interpretation of the same results.

A study of 100 university students has found that shorter sleep and delayed ability to get to sleep are associated with repeated negative thoughts (RNT). RNT are unwanted, unhelpful and distressing thoughts that are repeated over and over again, such as “my life is pointless”.

RNT can be a common problem in people with mental health issues such as generalised anxiety disorder and obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD).

Students filled in surveys assessing their sleep patterns, mood, anxiety levels and how often they experienced RNT. There was a clear correlation between poor sleep quality and RNT but the “direction of travel” is unclear. Does poor sleep lead to RNT or does RNT lead to poor sleep?

It’s plausible that lack of sleep might make negative thoughts or mood worse, as it does for concentration. Similarly, it’s easy to imagine that worrying about issues can stop someone sleeping well.

If you are troubled by persistent insomnia and unwelcome thoughts that you feel you cannot control, see your GP. Talking therapies such as cognitive behavioural therapy can often help with both of these issues.

Where did the story come from?

The study was carried out by researchers from Binghamton University, US. Sources of funding were not reported.

The study was published in the peer-reviewed medical journal Cognitive Therapy and Research.

The Mail Online accurately reported on the study (and somewhat unusually for the Mail, put a positive spin on the results), though it did not make it clear that the study was conducted on apparently healthy student volunteers. Similarly, it did not highlight the most important limitation of the study, the possibility of reverse causation or what is known in academic circles as “the chicken egg problem”.

The Mail also reported “a spate of studies [that] suggested getting between seven and eight hours is essential for good health”. This refers to separate research, which we have not appraised. Consequently, we cannot comment on the accuracy of these specific statements.

That said, there is a general consensus of expert opinion that persistent lack of sleep can be detrimental for both your physical and mental health.

What kind of research was this?

This was a cross-sectional study that aimed to see if the student responses to a variety of questionnaires indicated any association between repetitive negative thinking (RNT) and sleep. RNT describes when a person has distressing or worrying thoughts that are repeated over and over again and are difficult to control.

RNT occurs in a variety of mental illnesses, including generalised anxiety disorder, major depressionpost-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD). It can also occur in people who do not currently have a mental illness and generally causes increased feelings of anxiety and a lowering of mood. The researchers wanted to explore the relationship between RNT and a lack of sleep or delay in getting to sleep.

As this was a cross-sectional study, it cannot prove causation. That is, whether poor sleep causes RNT or RNT causes poor sleep. Both scenarios seem plausible.

What did the research involve?

The researchers recruited 100 US university students who had volunteered to participate in any research studies. They were on average 19 years old and 58% of them were female.

The students completed a variety of questionnaires assessing their levels of worry, thought patterns, sleep patterns and whether they were more of a morning or an evening person. This included the:

Worry Domain Questionnaire (WDQ)

Ruminative Response Scale of the Response Style Questionnaire (RRS)

Obsessive Compulsive Inventory (OCI)

Perseverative Thinking Questionnaire (PTQ)

Positive and Negative Affective Schedule (PANAS)

Negative Affect scale (PANAS-NA)

Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI)

Horne Ostberg Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire (MEQ)

The researchers then performed statistical analyses to look for any associations between sleep and repetitive negative thinking from the answers to these questionnaires.

 

What were the basic results?

The main findings were:

increased RNT was associated with shorter sleep and delayed sleep

shorter sleep was associated with more rumination (repetitive thinking)

delayed time getting to sleep was associated with more obsessive-compulsive symptoms

On average, the students:

went to bed at 1am and got to sleep within 22 minutes

slept for about seven hours

were mostly “evening” types

did not score highly overall for the symptoms on any of the questionnaires

How did the researchers interpret the results?

The conclusion reached by the researchers was that repetitive negative thinking “may be uniquely related to both sleep duration and timing”.

Conclusion

This study has found an association between shorter sleep and increased reported RNT.

However, there are a few points to bear in mind when considering how applicable the results of this study would be to the general population, people who have a mental illness or are particularly affected by RNT:

due to the cross-sectional measure of sleep patterns at one point in time, we cannot tell whether lack of sleep, or delayed sleep, causes RNT or whether RNT causes sleep disturbance – both directions of effect are plausible

none of the participants in the study were reported to be suffering from any mental illness or other conditions that may affect the level of RNT

they were all young, adult students

it could be argued that they may have been of a certain personality type to have been willing to complete seven extensive questionnaires

sleep patterns of people in this particular age group who are at university are unlikely to be representative of the sleep patterns they will have at other times of their life

However, commonsense tells us that a lack of sleep is likely to make any negative thoughts or mood worse. Tips on how to get a better night’s sleep can be found here.

If you are suffering from unwanted, repetitive thoughts that are causing you distress, talk to a healthcare professional. There are a range of simple techniques that can help, in addition to more formal methods such as cognitive behavioural therapy.

Autism and Sleep Issues- some great tips for helping a person on the autism spectrum to get a good night’s sleep

Autism and Sleep Issues

Autism and Sleep Issues

Jonathan Tarbox shares effective strategies for dealing with sleep challenges for children on the Autism spectrum. While there are effective strategies that work to change difficult sleep habits, unfortunately most parents remain unaware of what they are. Dr. Tarbox explains in simple language the importance of intervention, support and follow through.

Autism Live is a production of the Center for Autism and Related Disorders (CARD), headquartered in Tarzana, California, and with offices throughout, the United States and around the globe.